A Division of Learning Skills Research Foundation

 

The Maths Anxiety Trust aims to eliminate anxiety about Maths



THE MATH ANXIETY-PERFORMANCE LINK: A GLOBAL PHENOMENON

 

By Alana E. Foley (1), Julianne B. Herts (1), Francesca Borgonovi (2), Sonia Guerriero (2), Susan C. Levine (1), and Sian L. Beilock (1)

 

(1) Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, and (2) Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development, Paris, France.

 


Abstract

Demand for science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) professionals is on the rise worldwide. To effectively meet this demand, many governments and private organizations have revamped STEM education and promoted training to enhance math and science skills among students and workers. Education and training programs typically focus on increasing individuals’ math and science knowledge. However, data from laboratory studies and large-scale international assessments suggest that fear or apprehension about math, math anxiety, should also be considered when trying to increase math achievement and, in turn, STEM career success. This article reviews findings that shed light on antecedents of math anxiety, the bidirectional math anxiety-performance relation, underlying mechanisms, and promising routes to mitigating the negative relation between math anxiety and math performance.
The Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) Survey of Adult Skills reveals that numeracy skills are used extensively in work settings worldwide. Across participating OECD countries, 38% of workers aged 16 to 65 report using fractions at work at least once a week, 29% simple algebra or formulas, and 4% advanced math (OECD, 2013a). Moreover, the demand for science, technology, engineering, and math (STEM) professionals is predicted to increase. Consequently, many countries are interested in enhancing STEM education (BBC, 2013; Lacey & Wright, 2009).

To equip students with high levels of math and science knowledge, increased attention is being devoted to understanding why some countries are better at advancing math and science achievement than others (Mullis et al., 2012; OECD, 2009; Shimizu & Kaur, 2013). Much of this work has been driven by the idea that learning is a function of instruction time and quality. Accordingly, researchers have sought to understand how to maximize these factors (Dettmers, Trautwein, Lüdtke, Kunter, & Baumert, 2010; Scheerens & Bosker, 1997). However, recent evidence points to an often-ignored factor that may shape how well students are able to benefit from learning opportunities: math anxiety—the fear of, or apprehension about, math (Ashcraft & Kirk, 2001).

Data from the Program for International Student Assessment (PISA), which tests 15-year-olds’ academic achievement worldwide, shows that math anxiety is negatively related to math performance both within and across countries. In 63 of the 64 education systems that participated in PISA in 2012, students reporting higher levels of math anxiety displayed lower levels of math performance than their peers who reported lower levels of math anxiety (OECD, 2013b). Averaging this effect across participating countries, a one-unit increase in the PISA math-anxiety index for a given student corresponds to a 29-point decrease in his or her math score (medium effect size, Cohen’s d = 0.32; OECD, 2013b).

Countries with higher-than-average student math performance also tend to have lower-than-average student math anxiety (OECD, 2013b; Fig. 1). For example, Switzerland is above the mean of participating countries’ math performance (0.34 SD) and below the mean in math anxiety (−0.32 SD). At the other extreme, math achievement in Thailand is below the mean in math performance (−0.81 SD) and above the mean in math anxiety (0.55 SD). On average, accounting for GDP, a 1-unit difference in country-level math anxiety corresponds to a 73-point score gap on the math assessment (large effect size, Cohen’s d = 0.81).

In contrast to the above-mentioned pattern, many high-performing East Asian countries, where average student math achievement ranges from 0.76 to 2.13 SDs above the mean, also have math anxiety levels higher than the mean (0.44–1.41 SDs above the mean). Yet even these countries as a group (comprising Korea, Japan, Vietnam, Singapore, and regions of China) show a negative relation between country-level math anxiety and math performance that is statistically indistinguishable from the relation seen among the other participating countries (Fig. 1). A negative relation between math anxiety and math achievement is also seen among students within each of these East Asian countries.

PISA data show that math anxiety and math achievement are negatively related for students across the globe (OECD, 2013b). However, most of our understanding of the sources of math anxiety and the nature of its relation to math achievement comes from experimental studies (mainly in North America). Here, we consider how PISA data can provide external validity to laboratory findings and, in turn, how laboratory findings can elucidate the potential mechanisms and direction of causality underlying the negative relation between math anxiety and math performance identified in PISA.

The Bidirectional Relation Between Math Anxiety and Math Performance
Although there is consistent evidence of a negative relation between math anxiety and math performance, the directionality of this relation remains unclear. Does students’ math anxiety impair their ability to do math, do students develop math anxiety as a consequence of a preexisting difficulty with math, or is the relation bidirectional?

Before answering these questions, it is important to point out that although math anxiety is correlated with test anxiety and more general trait anxiety, intercorrelations between different measures of math anxiety are stronger than those between math anxiety and test or general anxiety (cf. Dowker, Sarkar, & Looi, 2016). Additionally, math anxiety is predictive of math test performance even when test anxiety is controlled for (cf. Lukowski et al., 2016). Similarly, when students simply anticipate doing math, those high in math anxiety show greater activity in brain regions associated with visceral threat detection and the experience of pain (dorsoposteriorinsula) than those low in math anxiety (Lyons & Beilock, 2012). This is not the case when those high in math anxiety anticipate doing a reading activity. Thus, math anxiety appears to be specific to the domain of math and does not necessarily imply anxiety related to other academic domains, such as reading.

Returning to the question of directionality, laboratory findings suggest that math anxiety leads to poor math performance but do not rule out the possibility that the relation is bidirectional. Behavioral (Ashcraft & Kirk, 2001; Park, Ramirez, & Beilock, 2014) and fMRI (Lyons & Beilock, 2011, 2012; Young, Wu, & Menon, 2012) studies suggest that math anxiety creates worries that can deplete resources in working memory—a cognitive system responsible for short-term storage and manipulation of information (Miyake & Shah, 1999) that is important for learning and performing well in math (Beilock & Carr, 2005; Raghubar, Barnes, & Hecht, 2010).

In a landmark study, Aschraft and Kirk (2001) demonstrated the disruptive impact math anxiety can have on working memory. They showed that students who are highest in math anxiety perform somewhat worse than their low-anxiety peers on complex addition problems but markedly worse on the same problems when simultaneously asked to hold a group of letters in mind for later recall. If math-anxious students must juggle math- related worries when their working memory is taxed (e.g., when a letter task is added to the mix, or when problems are difficult), their performance suffers.

Researchers have also explored overlaps between neural activity when students are doing math and the neural activity associated with negative emotions. When doing basic arithmetic, high-math-anxiety 7- to 9-year- olds show more activity in the right amygdala (implicated in processing negative emotions) than their low-math- anxiety peers (Young et al., 2012). This increased amygdala activity is accompanied by reduced activity in regions known to support working memory and numerical processing (e.g., dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, posterior parietal lobe). As negative responses to math increase, the resources necessary for successful math performance decrease.

Counterintuitively, students with the highest working memory capacity show the strongest negative relation between math anxiety and math performance (Beilock & Carr, 2005; Ramirez, Gunderson, Levine, & Beilock, 2013; Vukovic, Kieffer, Bailey, & Harari, 2013). When high-working-memory first and second graders face anxiety- inducing situations, they tend to rely on inefficient strategies that are less demanding of working memory or to make mistakes when trying to execute more advanced strategies (Ramirez, Chang, Maloney, Levine, & Beilock, 2016).

Moreover, high-working-memory, high-math-anxiety first- and second-grade students learn less math over the school year than their high-working-memory, low-math- anxiety peers (Ramirez et al., 2013). Conversely, math anxiety is not related to low-working-memory students’ math learning at this age (Ramirez et al., 2013; Vukovic et al., 2013; Wang & Shah, 2014). While the math performance of the high-working-memory students remains better than that of their low-working-memory peers, the gap between these groups is narrowed by math anxiety.

Data from PISA parallel laboratory studies showing that the negative relation between math anxiety and math performance is strongest for high-working-memory students. A comparison of the anxiety-performance relation for students at the top and bottom of the math- performance distribution reveals that the highest-achieving students—who are likely to have high working memory capacity—show the most striking negative relation between math anxiety and math performance across PISA countries (OECD, 2013b). On average, across countries, a 1-point increase in math anxiety was associated with a larger decrease in performance for students at the 90th percentile in math performance than students at the 10th percentile (medium effect sizes, Cohen’s ds = 0.32 and 0.25, respectively; Fig. 2).

Thus, experimental studies and PISA results show that students with higher potential to succeed in math are at greater risk of not reaching their full potential if they are math anxious.

While math anxiety can impair math performance by depleting working memory resources, difficulty with math may contribute to the development of math anxiety as well. For example, laboratory studies show that math- anxious individuals have difficulty with basic math tasks that are typically learned before elementary school entry, such as judging the magnitudes of pairs of numbers (Maloney, Ansari, & Fugelsang, 2011; Maloney, Risko, Ansari, & Fugelsang, 2010; Núñez-Peña & Suárez-Pellicioni, 2015). A poor grasp of basic math concepts may predispose students to develop math anxiety, partly in response to their math struggles (Levine, Gunderson, Maloney, Ramirez, & Beilock, 2015). Thus, the relation between math anxiety and math performance is likely bidirectional.

Antecedents of Math Anxiety

In addition to the idea discussed above that poor early math skills contribute to the development of math anxiety, findings point to several possible factors that can influence the development of math anxiety: quantity and quality of parent and teacher math input, societal pressure, and stereotypes. PISA data show that nations differ widely in their average levels of student math anxiety, underscoring the role that cultural context plays in its emergence.

Several studies suggest that there is a connection between adult role models and children’s math anxiety and math achievement. In India, a study of nearly 600 students in Grades 5 through 10 showed that parents’ math anxiety had a strong positive relation to children’s math anxiety and that children’s math anxiety, in turn, was negatively related to their math performance (Soni & Kumari, 2015). Evidence from the United States suggests that children who interact with high-math-anxiety adults show impaired math performance relative to their peers.

Indeed, first graders with high-math-anxiety teachers learn less math over the school year than those with low- math-anxiety teachers (Beilock, Gunderson, Ramirez, & Levine, 2010). Additionally, children of high-math-anxiety parents show less growth in math by the end of the school year than their peers with low-math-anxiety parents—but only when their parents frequently help them with homework, potentially exposing them to math anxiety. Importantly, the finding that social interaction moderates the relation between parent math anxiety and child math performance indicates that this relation is not readily explained by genetics alone (Maloney, Ramirez, Gunderson, Levine, & Beilock, 2015).

Finally, competitive performance and testing environments can create anxiety about meeting math performance expectations (Ramirez & Beilock, 2011). Specifically, in high-performing Asian countries (China, South Korea, Japan), the high-stakes academic culture may induce pressure to perform well in math and anxiety about one’s ability to do so (Stankov, 2010). PISA data do indeed show cross-national differences in student math anxiety that may reflect differences rooted in culture rather than in the level of student math performance. For example, Switzerland and Japan have similar average math performance scores (531 and 536, respectively), but Swiss students report below-average levels of math anxiety, whereas Japanese students report above-average levels of math anxiety.

Mitigating the Negative Effect of Math Anxiety on Performance

The widespread negative relation between math anxiety and performance, combined with evidence that math anxiety can interfere with the cognitive resources needed to do math, demonstrates the importance of addressing math anxiety in order to advance math performance, and STEM achievement more broadly. Efforts to curb math anxiety need to include ways to treat it in those who already experience it as well as ways to prevent it in the first place.

Regarding ways to treat math anxiety, lab studies suggest that psychological techniques emphasizing self- regulation, emotional control, and reappraisal of physiological threat responses hold promise ( Jamieson, Mendes, Blackstock, & Schmader, 2010; Lyons & Beilock, 2011; Foley et al. Park et al., 2014).

For example, among participants who were planning to take the GRE, those who were told that physiological arousal improves performance showed significantly better performance on the math section of the GRE—in the lab and on the actual test—than control participants who were not told of the benefits of anxiety (Jamieson et al., 2010). This method may benefit high- math-anxiety students in particular—a hypothesis that can be addressed in future research. Additionally, interventions could aim to temporarily reduce students’ math anxiety at times when it poses the greatest threat to performance, such as immediately before an exam. Park and colleagues (2014) found that asking students to write about their thoughts and feelings prior to taking a math test lessened the performance gap between high- and low-math-anxiety individuals. Rather than eliminating the strong physiological reaction many students have to math, it may be just as useful to engage in the expression or reappraisal of these emotional responses or, perhaps, simply to reframe the physiological reaction.

When parents and teachers contribute to a student’s math anxiety, understanding the mechanism through which their anxiety affects students’ math performance can provide insights into the best way to prevent the inter- generational transmission of math anxiety. Math-anxious adults tend to have negative attitudes about math (Hembree, 1990) and experience math anxiety in circumstances as basic as reading simple math problems aloud (Ashcraft & Ridley, 2005).

It is possible that when math- anxious parents and teachers engage in math-related interactions with children, such as helping with children’s math homework, their negative affect and attitudes interfere with their ability to effectively communicate about math, which in turn can negatively impact students’ math learning and achievement (Maloney et al., 2015). Tools that structure math-anxious parents’ and teachers’ inter- actions with students around math could positively affect children’s math achievement, even if they do not reduce the parents’ own math anxiety.

Berkowitz et al. (2015) found that Bedtime Math, a math iOS application that guides parents in reading a daily passage and answering related math questions with their first-grade children, significantly boosted the math scores of children with math-anxious parents. Moreover, a recent neuroimaging study found that high-math-anxiety third graders who received intensive one-on-one tutoring showed a significant reduction of amygdala activity when doing math, which, in turn, was related to lower levels of math anxiety (Supekar, Iuculano, Chen, & Menon, 2015).

Conclusion
Despite often-touted differences in math education and performance across countries, the negative relation between math anxiety and math performance, and its strength among high-achieving students, is a cross- national phenomenon. Given the wide array of countries and cultures surveyed by PISA, the robustness of math anxiety’s negative relation to math performance is striking. Because lab studies have demonstrated that math anxiety compromises students’ ability to learn and per- form, it is critical to address this factor in order to support and build the STEM workforce.

Although lab studies and PISA data have highlighted the relation between math anxiety and performance, many questions remain. For example, how do math-anxious teachers and parents transmit math anxiety to children? High- and low-anxiety teachers may design their lessons differently, spend different amounts of time on math, differentially spark student interest in math, and/or respond differently to students’ questions and errors. Additionally, math-anxious teachers and parents may engage in more direct transmission of negative or fearful attitudes via negative comments about math or their own math ability. Nonverbal behaviors (e.g., facial expressions, gestures, tone of voice, body language) may also convey negative affect about math.

Further, although PISA data show that math anxiety is a cross-national problem, we know very little about the most effective ways to address this issue in different cultural contexts. Given cross-national differences in student math anxiety, it is unlikely that a one-size-fits-all approach to designing interventions would work. Numerous aspects of culture, including gender and race stereotypes, associations of math achievement with brilliance (Leslie, Cimpian, Meyer, & Freeland, 2015), and student, teacher, and parent mind-sets and expectations, may contribute to the levels and distribution of math anxiety within a cultural context (Gunderson, Ramirez, Levine, & Beilock, 2012). For example, with respect to gender, in most but not all of the countries participating in PISA (56 of 64), females show higher math anxiety than males (OECD, 2013b). A critical next step for future research is to examine the role of cultural factors in the origins, prevalence, and distribution of math anxiety and how best to intervene to alleviate its negative effects on math performance and STEM participation.

Recommended Reading

Ashcraft, M. H., & Ridley, K. S. (2005). (See References). A comprehensive review of the mechanism through which math anxiety impacts math performance.

Maloney, E., & Beilock, S. L. (2012). Math anxiety: Who has it, why it develops, and how to guard against it. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 404–406. doi:10.1016/ j.tics.2012.06.008. A brief overview of math anxiety, its antecedents, and how it can be alleviated.

Ramirez, G., Gunderson, E. A., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2013). (See References). Evidence of individual differences in the math anxiety-performance relation in young children.

The Math Anxiety-Performance Link 57

Stoet, G., Bailey, D. H., Moore, A. M., & Geary, D. C. (2016). Countries with higher levels of gender equality show larger national sex differences in mathematics anxiety and relatively lower parental mathematics valuation for girls. PLoS ONE, 11(4), e0153857. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0153857. A recent analysis of PISA data showing that females report higher math anxiety than males in many nations, but that this difference is positively related to nations’ gender equality.

Young, C. B., Wu, S. S., & Menon, V. (2012). (See References). The first documented evidence of the neural underpinnings of math anxiety.

Author Note

Alana E. Foley and Julianne B. Herts contributed equally to the writing of this manuscript and should be considered as joint first authors. The views expressed in this article represent the views of the individual authors and do not represent an official position of the Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.

Funding

We thank the Overdeck Family Foundation (grant to Susan C. Levine and Sian L. Beilock), the National Science Foundation Spatial Intelligence and Learning Center (Grants SBE-1041707 and SBE-0541957 to Susan C. Levine), the Heising-Simons Foundation Development and Research in Early Mathematics Education (DREME) Network (support to Susan Levine), and the U.S. Department of Education Institute of Education Sciences (Grant R305A110682 to Sian L. Beilock) for support of this work.


References
Ashcraft, M. H., & Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 224–237.
Ashcraft, M. H., & Ridley, K. S. (2005). Math anxiety and its cognitive consequences: A tutorial review. In J. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 315–327). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
BBC. (2013, March 26). Global migrants: Which are the most wanted professions? Retrieved from http://www.bbc.com/ news/business-21938085
Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2005). When high-powered people fail working memory and “choking under pressure” in math. Psychological Science, 16, 101–105.
Beilock, S. L., Gunderson, E. A., Ramirez, G., & Levine, S. C. (2010). Female teachers’ math anxiety affects girls’ math achievement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 107, 1860–1863.
Berkowitz, T., Schaeffer, M. W., Maloney, E. A., Peterson, L., Gregor, C., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2015). Math at home adds up to achievement in school. Science, 350, 196–198.
Dettmers, S., Trautwein, U., Lüdtke, O., Kunter, M., & Baumert, J. (2010). Homework works if homework quality is high: Using multilevel modeling to predict the development of achievement in mathematics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 467–482.
Dowker, A., Sarkar, A., & Looi, C. Y. (2016). Mathematics anxiety: What have we learned in 60 years? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article 508. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00508
Gunderson, E. A., Ramirez, G., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2012). The roles of parents and teachers in the development of gender-related math attitudes. Sex Roles, 66, 153– 166. doi:10.1007/s11199-011-9996-2
Hembree, R. (1990). The nature, effects, and relief of mathematics anxiety. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 21, 33–46.
Jamieson, J. P., Mendes, W. B., Blackstock, E., & Schmader, T. (2010). Turning the knots in your stomach into bows: Reappraising arousal improves performance on the GRE. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 208–212.
Lacey, T. A., & Wright, B. (2009). Employment outlook: 2008- 18: Occupational employment projections to 2018. Monthly Labor Review, 132, 82–123.
Leslie, S. J., Cimpian, A., Meyer, M., & Freeland, E. (2015). Expectations of brilliance underlie gender distributions across academic disciplines. Science, 347, 262–265.
Levine, S. C., Gunderson, E. A., Maloney, E., Ramirez, G., & Beilock, S. (2015, March). The role of parents in young children’s math learning: Cognitive and emotional factors. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research on Child Development, Philadelphia, PA.
Lukowski, S. L., DiTrapani, J., Jeon, M., Wang, Z., Schenker, V. J., Doran, M. M., . . . Petrill, S. A. (2016). Multidimensionality in the measurement of math-specific anxiety and its relationship with mathematical performance. Learning and Individual Differences. Advance online publication. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2016.07.007
Lyons, I. M., & Beilock, S. L. (2011). Mathematics anxiety: Separating the math from the anxiety. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 2102–2110.
Lyons, I. M., & Beilock, S. L. (2012). When math hurts: Math anxiety predicts pain network activation in anticipation of doing math. PLoS ONE, 7(10), e48076. doi:10.1371/journal .pone.0048076.
Maloney, E. A., Ansari, D., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2011). The effect of mathematics anxiety on the processing of numerical magnitude. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 10–16.
Maloney, E. A., Ramirez, G., Gunderson, E. A., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2015). Intergenerational effects of parents’ math anxiety on children’s math achievement and anxiety. Psychological Science, 26, 1480–1488.
Maloney, E. A., Risko, E. F., Ansari, D., & Fugelsang, J. (2010). Mathematics anxiety affects counting but not subitizing during visual enumeration. Cognition, 114, 293–297.
Miyake, A., & Shah, P. (1999). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Minnich, C. A., Stanco, G. M., Arora, A., Centurino, V. A. S., & Castle, C. E. (Eds.). 58 Foley et al. (2012).
TIMSS 2011 encyclopedia: Education policy and curriculum in mathematics and science, Volumes 1 and 2. Chestnut Hill, MA: TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center, Boston College. Retrieved from http://timssand pirls.bc.edu/timss2011/encyclopedia-timss.html
Núñez-Peña, M. I., & Suárez-Pellicioni, M. (2015). Processing of multi-digit additions in high math-anxious individuals: Psychophysiological evidence. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 1268. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01268
Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development. (2009). Top of the class – High performers in science in PISA 2006. Paris, France: Author.
Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development. (2013a). OECD skills outlook 2013: First results from the survey of adult skills. Paris, France: Author.
Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development. (2013b). PISA 2012 results: Ready to learn: Students’ engagement, drive and self-beliefs (Vol. III). Paris, France: Author. doi:10.1787/9789264201170-en
Park, D., Ramirez, G., & Beilock, S. L. (2014). The role of expressive writing in math anxiety. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 20, 103–111.
Raghubar, K., Barnes, M. A., & Hecht, S. A. (2010). Working memory and mathematics: A review of developmental individual difference, and cognitive approaches. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 110–122.
Ramirez, G., & Beilock, S. L. (2011). Writing about testing worries boosts exam performance in the classroom. Science, 331, 211–213.
Ramirez, G., Chang, H., Maloney, E. A., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2016). On the relationship between math anxiety and math achievement in early elementary school: The role of
problem solving strategies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 83–100.
Ramirez, G., Gunderson, E. A., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2013). Math anxiety, working memory, and math achievement in early elementary school. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14, 187–202.
Scheerens, J., & Bosker, R. (1997). The foundations of educational effectiveness. New York, NY: Elsevier.
Shimizu, Y., & Kaur, B. (2013). Learning from similarities and differences: A reflection on the potentials and constraints of cross-national studies in mathematics. ZDM, 45(1), 1–5.
Soni, A., & Kumari, S. (2015). The role of parental math attitude in their children’s math achievement. International Journal of Applied Sociology, 5, 159–163.
Stankov, L. (2010). Unforgiving Confucian culture: A breeding ground for high academic achievement, test anxiety and self- doubt? Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 555–563.
Supekar, K., Iuculano, T., Chen, L., & Menon, V. (2015). Remediation of childhood math anxiety and associated neural circuits through cognitive tutoring. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35, 12574–12583.
Vukovic, R. K., Kieffer, M. J., Bailey, S. P., & Harari, R. R. (2013). Mathematics anxiety in young children: Concurrent and longitudinal associations with mathematical performance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38, 1–10.
Wang, Z., & Shah, P. (2014). The effect of pressure on high- and low-working-memory students: An elaboration of the choking under pressure hypothesis. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 226–238.
Young, C. B., Wu, S. S., & Menon, V. (2012). The neurodevelopmental basis of math anxiety. Psychological Science, 23, 492–501.
The Maths Anxiety Trust © 2024